Published: Aug. 18, 2018 By

A closer look at geographic data finds no correlation between generally happy locales and rates of suicide, according to new research by CU Boulder and U of California Irvine


Several years ago, a research paperĢżĢżby finding that the happiest states in the United States also had the highest suicide rates.

This seemingly paradoxical conclusion caught the attention of a trio of researchers from the ĄÖ²„“«Ć½ Boulder and University of California Irvine for a different reason: could there be more to the story than the eye-catching findings suggested?

Wadsworth

Tim Wadsworth

By looking at counties instead of states and making a few other experimental tweaks, researchers put the newsy findings to the test. The results of their study,ĢżĢżin the Journal of Happiness Studies, contradict the earlier study, adding another layer to a growing body of research about how our geographic location affects our happiness and well-being.

ā€œOur basic finding was that we donā€™t see any relationship between the general level of happiness in a place and peopleā€™s likelihood of committing suicide,ā€ saidĢżTim Wadsworth, an associate sociology professor at CU Boulder and one of the studyā€™s co-authors.

Designing the experiment

°Õ³ó±šĢż, conducted by a team of economists and published in 2011, concluded that the happiest U.S. states also had the highest suicide rates. The economistsā€™ explanation for this paradox was that people tended to compare themselves to those around themā€”if youā€™re an unhappy person in a happy place, your negative feelings might be exacerbated by your positive surroundings, which could lead to suicide.

Wadsworth, along with then-CU Boulder graduate studentĢżPhilip PendergastĢżand UC Irvine criminology professorĢż, were skeptical of the studyā€™s focus on people living in the same state.Ģż

Since states can span hundreds of thousands of square miles, there are major regional differences to considerā€”Boulder, for example, is very different from Colorado Springs. Positing that people were more likely to compare themselves to others in their immediate vicinity, the researchers narrowed in on data at the county level instead.

ā€œWe found it to be problematic, unless youā€™re going to make the argument that people in one part of California are comparing themselves to people in other parts of California,ā€ Wadsworth said. ā€œBut I will buy the idea that Iā€™m influenced by what I see living in Boulder among other Boulderites.ā€

They deviated from the initial study in other ways, too, such as by controlling for regional variations in suicide rates and well-being. The researchers also accounted for the availability of firearms and included a measure of overall health in their analysis, since those two factors are also correlated with suicide. In essence, they created a more rigorous experiment to see if they could reproduce the initial findings.

The idea that anybody could move to a place to change their level of happiness, thereā€™s no evidence that thatā€™s actually happening."

In the end, their study failed to find evidence of a relationship, either negative or positive, between suicide rates and the average subjective well-being, or happiness, of U.S. counties. Instead, the findings reinforced the traditional predictors of suicide, such as the accessibility of firearms and health and regional variations.Ģż

Noting the limitations of their work and the low number of studies on this topic, the researchers also pointed out the need for future research.

ā€œThe question is then, ā€˜Well, whoā€™s right?ā€™ā€ Kubrin said. ā€œNo study is ever the full explanation of anything. What awaits is additional studies to weigh in on thisā€”thatā€™s the way science works. More than anything, our paper raises questions about those initial findings.ā€

Understanding the findings

Practically speaking, the studyā€™s findings suggest that you shouldnā€™t necessarily be concerned about living in a happy place if youā€™re unhappy, nor that you should rush out and move to a happy place in an attempt to become happier.

ā€œThe idea that anybody could move to a place to change their level of happiness, thereā€™s no evidence that thatā€™s actually happening,ā€ Wadsworth said. ā€œLetā€™s not all of a sudden jump on the bandwagon and say we need to be extra concerned about unhappy people in happy places, not to say that itā€™s impossible, but (this study was) really just sort of holding up our hands and saying, ā€˜Whoa.ā€™ā€

Another analysis made headlines recently, too, this one from the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. In May, the federal agency reported that suicide rates across the country increased 25 percent between 1999 and 2016.

While that statistic alone is alarming, sociologists like Wadsworth and Kubrin say itā€™s important for public health officials and policymakers to drill down into the data to look at state, regional and local trends, as well as other factors such as race, age, education level, marital status and the availability of firearms.

ā€œOne of the things we know about suicide, as well as homicide, is that itā€™s not equally distributed,ā€ Kubrin said. ā€œOne of the things you have to do is disaggregate the suicide rate. The overall suicide rate is helpful, but not nearly as helpful as looking at different groups of people and the areas where the increase has occurred in particular.ā€

On the other side of the spectrum, when our friends, family members and even celebrities such as Kate Spade and Anthony Bourdain die by suicide, we tend to focus on their individual circumstances, such as their mental health. But if we really want to prevent more suicides across the country, itā€™s equally as important to look at widescale trends that go beyond individual factors such as depression, Kubrin said.

Pendergastkubrin

Philip Pendergast and Charis Kubrin

ā€œIndividual-level explanations are 100 percent absolutely important,ā€ she said. ā€œBut if we just stay at the individual level, we might be missing bigger, broader factors that impact suicide above and beyond any one individualā€™s characteristics. There are other questions about suicide that need answering. Why do suicide rates cluster in certain regions of the U.S.? Itā€™s not just a function of people with depression moving to certain areas. Thereā€™s something beyond the individual that plays into this distribution.ā€

Suicide research continues

Overall, the researchers say that further study on suicide is needed. Though suicide is much more common than homicide or violent crime, it receives a fraction of the attention.

ā€œWeā€™re obsessed with crime and crime trends and crime data,ā€ Kubrin said. ā€œThe reality is that suicide, especially for certain groups, is much more likely to occur than homicide.ā€

For his part, Pendergast said he hopes to continue his contributions to suicide research in his new role as administrator of the U.S. Census Bureauā€™s Rocky Mountain Federal Statistical Research Data Center, which is housed on campus.

After completing his doctorate at CU in May, Pendergast now reviews proposals from people who want to use the centerā€™s restricted population data in their research. With such a rich dataset, Pendergast said he hopes researchers will explore topics such as why the American West has a higher suicide rate than other parts of the country. More broadly, Pendergast said he hopes this and other studies help spark much-needed conversations about suicide.

ā€œI hope that any papers about suicide would have the implication of causing people to engage in more of a cultural dialogue about suicide and making it less stigmatized by simply talking about it,ā€ Pendergast said.